Authors:
|
Emily Brown
|
Mary Haghshenas
|
Scott Geib
|
Rebecca DeGraaf
|
Abstract
|
The pluot is a complex hybrid, genetically made up
of 75 percent plum and 25 percent apricot. According to our group's
observations on flavor, the taste of the pluot is more similar to that
of a plum than an apricot, and we therefore hypothesized that the chemical
makeup of carbohydrates and enzymes residing in this fruit would be
very similar to a plum. Samples were taken by producing extract of each
fruit from flesh and skin. These were used for carbohydrate analysis
in Barfoed's and Selivanoff's test, testing for monosaccaride reducing
sugars and aldehyde vs keytone structure respectively. Both fruits gave
similar results for each test showing monosaccaride reducing sugars
and keytone structures present. We also conducted paper chromatography
with each of the fruit pigment extracts, finding each to have one pigment
separate out with similar color and Rf value, suggesting the same pigment
present and similar pigment composition. Because the plum and pluot
have very different pH's, 5.0 in plum and 3.0 in pluot, we tested for
activity of the enzyme polyphenoloxidase (PPO) from each fruit at different
pH values. We found that there seemed to be a shift in optimal pH for
greatest enzyme activity to a slightly lower pH in the pluot than the
plum, suggesting that there may be a slight difference in what we called
PPO in the plum from the pluot. From all of this evidence, we concluded
that the plum and pluot are almost identical in the aspects that we
tested for.
|
Figure 10 |
Discussion How similar are pluots, a hybrid from 75% plum and 25% apricots, to
their plum parent that they taste so similar to? We set out to see if
this new hybrid fruit really had any major differences from a plum,
because when we tasted them side by side, we could not tell any difference
in taste. Are they just overpriced plums with a fancy name and speckled
skin? We decided to compare them in three different aspects. First,
how are the structures of their carbohydrates similar or different?
Also, their skins look very different, so are there different pigments
in their skin cells? Finally, we knew that the pluot has a much lower
pH, so would the enzyme polyphenoloxidase have higher activity at lower
pH values in the pluot than the plum? We came into this experiment believing
that they should be very similar is all of these aspects based on our
taste test. In our first experiment we ran 3 trials for both Barfoed's and Selivanoff's
tests for each extract and got positive results for both fruits at both
10% and 15% concentrations suggesting that monosaccharide reducing sugars
and ketose structures are present in each (Figures 1 and 2, Tables 1
and 2). We based our positive results on sugar samples that we already
knew the structures of. We ran glucose as a known positive because it
is an aldose monosaccharide reducing sugar. For a negative result we
ran sucrose, a disaccharide containing a ketose monomer. For Barfoed's test we took the first trial's samples and took the absorption
of them after dilution to try to quantify the positive result. We only
chose to do the absorbance of one of the trials because all three showed
very similar results. We found that all of the samples had low absorption
at 400nm (violet) and 550 nm (green) and very high absorption at 700
nm (red) (Figure 4, Table3). The low absorption at the low end of the
spectrum makes sense, because the solutions were obviously blue and
all wavelengths at or around blue would be mostly transmitted. Since
blue light is at around 450-500 nm, this correlates with our data. Also,
the fruit extract samples had the highest absorption values and they
were closest to sucrose, suggesting some correlation between all the
positive samples. At the higher wavelength of 700, which is red light,
we had hoped to show a lower absorption for the pigments and sucrose
which all contained a red precipitate, but this was not seen in our
data. Although we vortexed each tube before reading the absorbance to
keep the precipitate from settling, we still could not quantify it.
We believe this is because the precipitate was at such a low concentration,
once resuspended in the solution after vortexing, it could not be picked
up by the spectrophotometer. Despite this, we still consider all of
the extracts to show positive for presence of monosaccharide reducing
sugars because we could see an obvious red precipitate in all three
of our trials, and see our attempt to quantify the precipitate as a
poor procedure for doing so. To correct our poor procedure in the future,
it would be better to pull off as much of the supernatant of the samples
as possible and take the absorbance readings of the precipitate in a
higher concentration from the supernatant. Also, we saw little difference
from the low concentration (10%) solutions and the high concentration
(15%) (Table3). There was a slight trend that the low concentrations
showed higher absorbance at the low wavelengths and lower absorbance
at high wavelengths than the high concentrations, but the variance between
them was not large enough to justify a difference between the two samples.
We believe that this is because the concentrations are too similar,
and an improvement that could be made in the future would be to vary
the concentrations more, possibly having one be twice the other (for
example, if we repeated this we would use 10% and 20% or 7.5% and 15%).
We consider both extracts to show very similar results for Barfoed's
test and consider our hypothesis that they would have the same composition
valid in the aspect of monosaccharide reducing sugars. In Selivanoff's test, we saw all trials of all extracts at both concentrations
show positive results for presence of ketoses (Figure 2). This was based
off of the time that they took to turn color. They did not turn as quickly
as sucrose, which changed at 15 seconds, but still were well under one
minute for both concentrations, which was the listed time as the dividing
point between a positive and negative result. Between the two fruit
extracts, the plum began to change color the fastest, averaging 20 seconds
in the three trials at the high concentration (Figure 3, Table 2). Also,
the higher concentration, on average changed faster than the low concentration
in the plum, suggesting a relationship between the concentration of
sugars containing ketoses and speed at which the color change occurs.
The pluot, on average, changed color slower than the plum. It averaged
30 seconds at the 15% concentration and 37 seconds at the 10% concentration
(Figure 3, Table 2). The pluot showed the same differences in time between
concentration, strengthening the evidence that there is a relationship
between the concentration of sugars containing ketoses and speed at
which the color change occurs. Because of this, and the fact that the
pluot changed color consistently slower on average than the plum leads
us to suggest that the plum has a higher concentration of sugars containing
ketose groups than the pluot. This test showed presence in both fruits
of ketose sugars, but in different concentrations, which begins to refute
our hypothesis that they would be similar in all aspects of the carbohydrate
tests that we would run. We chose not to take absorbance of our positive results for Selivanoff's
test because the strength of color change was not the factor that determined
the difference between positive and negative results. Instead it is
the time that was the important factor. From our testing each extract
at different concentrations and finding faster color changes at higher
concentrations leads us to believe that time can be used as a quantitative
value of what concentration of the sugars in a solution are ketose.
This is what allows us to suggest that the plum contains a higher concentration
of ketose sugars than the pluot. We cannot conclude what sugars are
in each of the fruits, or say that all the sugars are ketose and monosaccharide
reducing sugars. We can only say that some of the sugars in each of
the fruits are monosaccharide reducing and some are ketose. In the paper chromatography, in both trials for both fruits we found
one pigment present on the paper chromatography strip (Figures 5 and
6). For all of the trials, the pigment had a similar yellow color and
the Rf values for all of the trials were very close. For the plum, the
average Rf value between the two trials was 0.97 with the two trials
varying little (.007 from the average or 0.7%) (Table 4). For the pluot
the values varied even less (.002 from the average or 0.2%) with the
average being 0.968 (Table 4). The average Rf values of the two fruit
pigment extracts for the yellow pigment were also very close, varying
by only 0.002, or approximately 0.2% (Table 4). This leads us to believe
that the pigment extracted from each fruit in each trial is the same
pigment. We cannot conclude what pigment this is, but comparing it to
the previous week's experiment where spinach chloroplasts were extracted
and the same chromatography test was done, a pigment similar in color
was seen, xanthophyll, but the Rf value that was calculated for it had
an average of 0.84, which is too different from the Rf value of this
pigment to conclude that they are the same (Table 4). This data supports
our hypothesis that the pigments of each fruit are similar in the aspects
we tested, despite their obvious color differences. We also conducted an absorption spectrum of each of the fruit pigment
extracts. Our intentions here were to see what colors of light were
absorbed most in each extraction, or more simply what colors each extract
was made of. Wavelengths that had low absorbance would have high transmittance
in the extracts and those would be the colors you would see in the solution.
Our results show similar absorbencies for each of the pigment extract
with the highest absorption at the lower wavelengths and the lowest
absorption at the higher wavelengths showing more or less a negative
linear regression (Figure 7, Table 5). This data would suggest that
the pigments are made up primarily of red pigments with an intermediate
concentration of green pigments, and the lowest concentration of blue
and violet pigments. This correlates with the colors of our original
pigment extract solutions, because both appeared reddish, with the pluot
looking orangey/peach, and the plum was rose colored. Both of the pigment
extracts showed very similar trends through the absorbance spectrum,
suggesting similar pigment composition and further supporting our hypothesis.
We found that the pH value for our pluots were much lower than our
plums. The pH of our plums was 5.0 and our pluots were 3.0 (Figure 9).
This led us to wonder if the enzyme activity for each fruit would be
at greatest activity at the pH that the fruit is naturally found. We
tested and found presence of the enzyme polyphenoloxidase (PPO) in each
of the fruits, and decided to take extracts from each fruit containing
this enzyme and subject them to varying pH level (Figure 8). Then we
would activate the enzyme and allow it equal time to react in each extract.
The absorbance of each extract was taken at each pH, with high absorbance
representing high PPO activity. We found that the plum had highest absorbance
(meaning enzyme activity) in a pH buffer of 8.0 (Figure 10, Table 6).
The pluot had the highest activity at a pH between 7.0-7.5, slightly
lower than the plum (Figure 10, Table 6). This shift downward of the
peak enzyme activity could be because of the pluot having a lower ph
than then plum, but does not fully support it. We conducted two trials
of this test for each fruit and saw similar trends for each trial. We
would have conducted this same test at much lower pH level that are
equal to that of the fruits, but there was not any buffer solution available
below a pH of 5.0. If peak enzyme activity was seen at the actual pH
values of the fruit, then it would support more strongly that the enzyme
activity of the pluot is highest at lower pH values than the plum. Still,
our data shows differences in both the pH and the enzyme activity at
specific pH values between the two fruits tested, going against our
hypothesis tested and ultimately causing us to refute our hypothesis.
All of our tests suggested that the plum and pluot are very similar
in structure in many ways, but also suggests that there are certain
aspects in which the fruits differ. In the carbohydrate tests run, both
fruits showed presence for both ketose and monosaccharide reducing sugar
structures, but we could conclude that the plum had a greater concentration
of ketose sugars than the pluot based on the reaction time of each.
In the tests based off of the pigment of the fruits, we could separate
out only one pigment for each fruit, and believe that it is the same
pigment in each of the fruits. We also saw similar pigment composition
in the absorbance spectrum of each of the extracts. We see both of these
results as possibly erroneous, because we could see obvious differences
in skin color between the two fruits, and would have thought that they
contain different pigmentation. Finally, we found that the fruits had
very different pH values with the pluot having the lower, and also that
the enzyme polyphenoloxidase had the greatest activity at a lower pH
value in the pluot. Because of these factors, we are forced to refute
our hypothesis that both of the fruits would be the same in the aspects
that we are testing for, and we now understand that they do have some
fundamental differences in organic composition. This information is important because it allows us to begin to understand
that hybridized and genetically alter foods have differences from the
foods they are derived from. We only found small differences between
our fruits, but some modifications could cause much larger changes that
could have negative effects. A hybridized fruit could become more susceptible
to pathogens or parasites, making it uneconomical to grow. Even more
serious is that the hybrid could carry diseases, mutations, or allergies
that the person eating is unaware of. Anything is possible in the realm
of genetics. By seeing our small changes that occurred in the few things
we tested for makes us realize the dangers of modifying our foods and
suggests we should be cautious in the changes we make from the natural
world. |